OpenAlex Citation Counts

OpenAlex Citations Logo

OpenAlex is a bibliographic catalogue of scientific papers, authors and institutions accessible in open access mode, named after the Library of Alexandria. It's citation coverage is excellent and I hope you will find utility in this listing of citing articles!

If you click the article title, you'll navigate to the article, as listed in CrossRef. If you click the Open Access links, you'll navigate to the "best Open Access location". Clicking the citation count will open this listing for that article. Lastly at the bottom of the page, you'll find basic pagination options.

Requested Article:

Primate Cognition
Amanda M. Seed, Michael Tomasello
Topics in Cognitive Science (2010) Vol. 2, Iss. 3, pp. 407-419
Closed Access | Times Cited: 867

Showing 76-100 of 867 citing articles:

Mirror neurons: Functions, mechanisms and models
Erhan Öztop, Mitsuo Kawato, Michael A. Arbib
Neuroscience Letters (2012) Vol. 540, pp. 43-55
Closed Access | Times Cited: 127

The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures
Marta Halina, Federico Rossano, Michael Tomasello
Animal Cognition (2013) Vol. 16, Iss. 4, pp. 653-666
Closed Access | Times Cited: 124

Unraveling the evolution of uniquely human cognition
Evan L. MacLean
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2016) Vol. 113, Iss. 23, pp. 6348-6354
Open Access | Times Cited: 122

“Soft” policing at hot spots—do police community support officers work? A randomized controlled trial
Barak Ariel, Cristóbal Weinborn, Lawrence W. Sherman
Journal of Experimental Criminology (2016) Vol. 12, Iss. 3, pp. 277-317
Open Access | Times Cited: 121

The Language Myth
Vyvyan Evans
(2014)
Closed Access | Times Cited: 120

Coalitions in theory and reality: a review of pertinent variables and processes
Annie Bissonnette, Susan Perry, Louise Barrett, et al.
Behaviour (2014) Vol. 152, Iss. 1, pp. 1-56
Open Access | Times Cited: 118

Competing iconicities in the structure of languages
Irit Meir, Carol Padden, Mark Aronoff, et al.
Cognitive Linguistics (2013) Vol. 24, Iss. 2, pp. 309-343
Open Access | Times Cited: 117

Mapping putative hubs in human, chimpanzee and rhesus macaque connectomes via diffusion tractography
Longchuan Li, Xiaoping Hu, Todd M. Preuss, et al.
NeuroImage (2013) Vol. 80, pp. 462-474
Open Access | Times Cited: 108

Human brain evolution: From gene discovery to phenotype discovery
Todd M. Preuss
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (2012) Vol. 109, Iss. supplement_1, pp. 10709-10716
Open Access | Times Cited: 108

Basis for Cumulative Cultural Evolution in Chimpanzees: Social Learning of a More Efficient Tool-Use Technique
Shinya Yamamoto, Tatyana Humle, Masayuki Tanaka
PLoS ONE (2013) Vol. 8, Iss. 1, pp. e55768-e55768
Open Access | Times Cited: 104

Is primate tool use special? Chimpanzee and New Caledonian crow compared
William C. McGrew
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences (2013) Vol. 368, Iss. 1630, pp. 20120422-20120422
Open Access | Times Cited: 102

Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality
Sandra A. Heldstab, Zaida K. Kosonen, Sonja E. Koski, et al.
Scientific Reports (2016) Vol. 6, Iss. 1
Open Access | Times Cited: 102

Citizen Science as a New Tool in Dog Cognition Research
Laughlin Stewart, Evan L. MacLean, D. Dunbar Ivy, et al.
PLoS ONE (2015) Vol. 10, Iss. 9, pp. e0135176-e0135176
Open Access | Times Cited: 101

The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures
Claudio Tennie, Elisa Bandini, Carel P. van Schaik, et al.
Biology & Philosophy (2020) Vol. 35, Iss. 5
Open Access | Times Cited: 99

Comparing physical and social cognitive skills in macaque species with different degrees of social tolerance
Marine Joly, Jérôme Micheletta, Arianna De Marco, et al.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences (2017) Vol. 284, Iss. 1862, pp. 20162738-20162738
Open Access | Times Cited: 93

The Measure of All Minds: Evaluating Natural and Artificial Intelligence
José Hernández‐Orallo
(2017)
Closed Access | Times Cited: 90

Levels of explainable artificial intelligence for human-aligned conversational explanations
Richard Dazeley, Peter Vamplew, Cameron Foale, et al.
Artificial Intelligence (2021) Vol. 299, pp. 103525-103525
Open Access | Times Cited: 90

Birds of a feather? Parrot and corvid cognition compared
Megan L. Lambert, Ivo Jacobs, Mathias Osvath, et al.
Behaviour (2018) Vol. 156, Iss. 5-8, pp. 505-594
Closed Access | Times Cited: 88

The Language Network Is Recruited but Not Required for Nonverbal Event Semantics
Anna A. Ivanova, Zachary Mineroff, Vitor Zimmerer, et al.
Neurobiology of Language (2021) Vol. 2, Iss. 2, pp. 176-201
Open Access | Times Cited: 60

Model-free metacognition
Peter Carruthers, David M. Williams
Cognition (2022) Vol. 225, pp. 105117-105117
Closed Access | Times Cited: 41

The prefrontal cortex: from monkey to man
Richard Lévy
Brain (2023) Vol. 147, Iss. 3, pp. 794-815
Open Access | Times Cited: 29

The evolution of comparative cognition: Is the snark still a boojum?
Sara J. Shettleworth
Behavioural Processes (2008) Vol. 80, Iss. 3, pp. 210-217
Closed Access | Times Cited: 135

Attention to attention in domestic dog (Canis familiaris) dyadic play
Alexandra Horowitz
Animal Cognition (2008) Vol. 12, Iss. 1, pp. 107-118
Closed Access | Times Cited: 133

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) do not develop contingent reciprocity in an experimental task
Sarah F. Brosnan, Joan B. Silk, Joseph Henrich, et al.
Animal Cognition (2009) Vol. 12, Iss. 4, pp. 587-597
Open Access | Times Cited: 130

From monkey mirror neurons to primate behaviours: possible ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ pathways
Pier Francesco Ferrari, Luca Bonini, Leonardo Fogassi
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences (2009) Vol. 364, Iss. 1528, pp. 2311-2323
Open Access | Times Cited: 119

Scroll to top